What is the term that refers to the differing complementary views from biological to psychological to socio cultural for analyzing any given phenomenon?

In order to continue enjoying our site, we ask that you confirm your identity as a human. Thank you very much for your cooperation.

Social and biological phenomena are widely recognized as determinants of human development, health, and socioeconomic attainments across the life course, but our understanding of the underlying pathways and processes remains limited. To address this gap, we define the “biosocial approach” as one that conceptualizes the biological and social as mutually constituting, and that draws on models and methods from the biomedical and social/behavioral sciences. By bringing biology into the social sciences, we can illuminate mechanisms through which socioeconomic, psychosocial, and other contextual factors shape human development and health. Human biology is a social biology, and biological measures can therefore identify aspects of social contexts that are harmful, as well as beneficial, with respect to well-being. By bringing social science concepts and study designs to biology and biomedicine, we encourage an epistemological shift that foregrounds social/contextual factors as important determinants of human biology and health. The biosocial approach also underscores the importance of the life course, as assessments of both biological and social features throughout human development over time, and across generations, are needed to achieve a full understanding of social and physical well-being. We conclude with a brief review of the papers in the volume, which showcase the value of a biosocial approach to understanding the pathways linking social stratification, biology, and health across the life course.

Keywords: human development, health disparities, socioeconomic status, social genomics

Social, cultural, economic, and biological factors are widely recognized as critical determinants of well-being across the life course. Yet an integrative understanding of the multilevel biosocial pathways linking society, biology, health, and socio-economic attainment remains elusive. The objective of this special issue is to showcase research that integrates theory, data, and methods from the social and biological sciences to advance our understanding of social and biological processes that contribute to, or derive from, social stratification across the life course. In this introduction we describe the state of current research, and discuss the motivation for, and relevant concepts underlying, a biosocial perspective. We review the themes and research contributions in this special issue, and chart a course forward for understanding biosocial pathways of well-being across the life course.

The term “biosocial” is widely used in the social sciences, but rarely defined. Perhaps its meaning is self-evident. And while the term has appeared in the scientific literature for more than fifty years (e.g., both the Journal of Biosocial Science and Social Biology1 began publishing in 1969), over the past 15 years there has been a qualitative shift in approaches and applications in biosocial research. In this section we discuss these developments, and the synergies afforded by integrating perspectives from the social and biological sciences.

We define “biosocial” as a broad concept referencing the dynamic, bidirectional interactions between biological phenomena and social relationships and contexts, which constitute processes of human development over the life course. It is difficult, if not impossible, to represent in two dimensions the complexities of these biosocial dynamics, but we attempt to do so in Figure 1, which builds on prior efforts (Kuh and Ben-Shlomo 2004; Hertzman and Power 2006; Glass and McAtee 2006) to highlight the multilevel domains and pathways of particular importance in biosocial approaches to health and social inequality. The top boxes represent the set of nested and interacting social contexts “outside” the body that impact the developing brain and body of an individual throughout all stages of the life course. Similarly, the bottom boxes represent the nested and interacting levels of biological organization “inside” the brain and body that respond to, and shape, social worlds. What constitutes “biological” can be characterized as processes and structures within an individual that contribute to the growth, reproduction, and maintenance of the soma from conception to death. Biology is typically organized across multiple levels, including the genome, molecular interactions (e.g., gene expression, hormone production), integrated physiological and neurological systems (e.g., the cardiovascular system; the sympathetic adrenal medullary axis), organs and other tissues, and cells and cellular processes.

Social phenomena are similarly complex and multidimensional, and are illustrated by the relationships and interactions among individuals living in groups and within social contexts (e.g., families, neighborhoods, schools) who share the norms, institutions, and hierarchies that structure them. The social realm can also include aspects of the physical environment of relevance to biology (e.g., exposure to environmental contaminants, public space for recreation), that are structured by social relations and hierarchies.

A biosocial perspective, therefore, draws on models and methods from the biological, medical, behavioral, and social sciences. It conceptualizes the biological and the social as mutually constituting forces, and blurs boundaries between phenomena inside the body and outside of the body. It implies that attempts to understand one without the other are incomplete. It is a transdisciplinary approach to understanding human development, behavior, and health, developed and applied by scholars that often have disciplinary backgrounds in anthropology, psychology, epidemiology, sociology, economics, public health, genomics, medicine, and demography.

Ongoing calls for a more integrative, multi-method, multilevel interdisciplinary approach to research on human development, health, and social inequality underscore the importance and potential contribution of a biosocial perspective (Council 2001a; 2001b; Halfon & Hochstein 2002; Harris 2010; Weinstein, Vaupel & Wachter 2007). The recent expansion of methodological options for collecting biological samples in non-clinical settings has facilitated this effort, and innovative biological measures are increasingly being incorporated into social science research designs and data collection efforts. A new generation of biosocial research is poised to bridge the gap between community- and clinic-based approaches to understanding the dynamic interplay of biology and social context across the life course.

Why should social and behavioral scientists care about biology? While we recognize that most, if not all, social and economic outcomes have some biological component, social scientists—with a few notable exceptions—have generally not considered biological processes with specificity or depth. This position does not always derive from theoretical or epistemological stances, and is often due to gaps in data, constraints of training and motivational structures that are set within disciplinary frameworks, and logistical challenges associated with collecting biological measures in non-clinical settings. Many of these gaps are narrowing.

There are several reasons why putting the “bio” in “biosocial” has the potential to make important contributions to the social and behavioral sciences. First, humans are biological creatures, embedded in families, social networks, communities, and cultures. Context matters to human biology, and engagement with biological concepts and measures reflects this reality. This is especially clear in the case of human health, where the importance of “social determinants” is well-established and widely known (Adler et al. 1994; Glass & McAtee 2006; Link & Phelan 1995), and where social impacts on underlying physiological processes are apparent and increasingly elaborated (Uchino, Cacioppo, & Kiecolt-Glaser 1996; Yang et al. 2016). Attention to biology has the potential to illuminate mechanisms through which socioeconomic, demographic, and psychosocial factors shape human development and health within the contexts of everyday life.

The importance of context to human biology is evident across multiple time dimensions (Lasker 1969). In the short term, homeostasis and allostasis—processes of adaptation to changes in current or anticipated environments (McEwen, 1998; Sterling & Ayer 1988)—facilitate physiological and/or behavioral responses to the shifting demands and opportunities of local environments. For example, a perceived danger or social threat increases the production of cortisol, a hormone that plays a central role in mobilizing the body’s response to stress. When the threat is removed, cortisol production returns to baseline (Gruenewald, Kemeny, Aziz, & Fahey 2004). But repeat, or chronic, exposure to adverse environmental conditions can reset regulatory set points, resulting in “wear and tear” on key physiological systems (Seeman, McEwen, Rowe, & Singer 2001). Lower socioeconomic status—a source of chronic stress—is associated with high cortisol in the evening, and a flatter rhythm of production across the day compared to the normative pattern of declining cortisol production over the day to low levels in the evening (Cohen et al. 2006; DeSantis et al. 2007). Longer term effects of environments on biological systems emerge from critical or sensitive periods of development, when exposures can have disproportionate, enduring effects on biological structure and function. Continuing with the example of cortisol, individuals born with a lower birth weight have elevated cortisol in adulthood (Phillips et al., 2000), pointing toward a biological mechanism through which lower socioeconomic status (a strong predictor of lower birth weight) may impact health within and across generations.

By getting “under the skin,” biological measures provide direct, objective information on pathophysiological processes that contribute to the emergence of disease, before clinically diagnosable disease is evident. For example, relative levels of blood pressure—a robust indicator of future risk of cardiovascular disease—tend to “track” from childhood into adulthood (Berenson, Wattigney, Bao, Srinivasan & Radhakrishnamurthy 1995; Li, Chen, Srinivasan & Berenson 2004). While measuring blood pressure in childhood or young adulthood will reveal few clinical cases of hypertension, it will identify individuals most at risk for the future development of cardiovascular disease and early death (Nguyen et al. 2011). Biological measures therefore enhance our understanding of how social environments influence pre-disease pathways, and provide opportunities for intervention prior to the emergence of clinical disease.

Attention to biology can also identify which aspects of social and physical environments are most detrimental to health and socio-economic well-being, as well as point toward resiliency and protective factors that buffer groups of individuals from the effects of adverse environments. The concept of “embodiment” has been invoked repeatedly in the social sciences to underscore the social and political nature of the human body, and its responsiveness to social and cultural context (Gravlee 2009; Krieger 2005; Scheper, Hughes & Lock 1987; Seligman 2014). The body tells stories—literally and figuratively—and biological measures offer opportunities to access information that reflects the quality of social environments. Recent work on “skin deep resilience” provides a case in point: Among African-Americans from low SES backgrounds, measures of self-control predict better psychosocial outcomes (for example, lower depression, lower likelihood of substance use), but worse physical health outcomes, as revealed by several biological measures (Brody et al. 2013; Miller et al. 2015). Biological measurement may therefore add an important dimension to our understanding of “health” (i.e., self-report, psychosocial, and biological measures may tell different “stories”), and they may be particularly useful in settings where accurate self-reports are especially difficult to obtain, as in research with children, or across international settings where linguistic and/or cultural factors may contribute to variation in perception, experience, and/or reporting (Hahn 1995; Kleinman 1986).

While social factors impact biological process and health outcomes, the reverse is also true. For example, lower birth weight—a biological variable reflecting the quality of the prenatal environment, which is in turn shaped by genetic, developmental, and social factors—has adverse effects on cognitive development and adult educational attainment (Conley & Bennett, 2000) Figlio et al. 2014). Level of education is also a partial function of inherited genotype, and common genetic factors can account for some of the well-established association between education and health (Boardman, Domingue, & Daw 2015); Okbay et al. 2016). Biological processes, therefore, influence individual life course trajectories, shape social and educational attainments, and inform selection into social and physical environments which can feedback onto biological processes. When scholars do not consider how biological mechanisms shape developmental outcomes, or interact with social environments to influence social stratification across the life course, models may be incomplete or mis-specified, parameter estimates of environmental effects overstated, and results biased.

A biosocial perspective is also important for translating social science research into policy. As noted above, biological measures can reveal the quality of social conditions, and in some cases these measures may motivate action to improve conditions to prevent disease, rather than treat individuals already on the path toward disease. For example, lead screening in children can be used to inform housing policy, where initiatives aimed at reducing lead exposure can prevent the development of costly cognitive and behavioral disorders. Consistent evidence on the importance of social relationships for biological processes affecting health suggests routine health screenings should include questions about the quantity and quality of individuals’ social connections, and physicians should be encouraged to ask their patients about their relationships as part of their annual wellness check-ups (Yang et al. 2016).

Biological measures can also add important dimensions to the evaluation of social policies. For example, the Moving to Opportunity (MTO) demonstration project was initiated in 1994 to investigate the impact of residential contexts on educational attainments, income, and overall well-being. Families in public housing were randomized into an experimental condition which subsidized their move into a low poverty neighborhood, while controls were not offered new assistance. The intervention had limited effects on education and income—the outcomes of primary interest when the study was designed—but large impacts on health: Assignment to the low poverty group resulted in a 13–19% reduction in obesity and 22% reduction in diabetes, in comparison with the control group (Ludwig et al. 2011). A biosocial approach to policy evaluation can identify the biological processes and pre-disease pathways that are affected by contextual factors like neighborhood poverty, and point toward social programs that improve health. Given the high costs of health care, this kind of information may add an important, but often overlooked, component to cost/benefit analyses of social policies.

The biosocial approach occupies an important and expanding space in the social and behavioral sciences, where the emphasis has been on integrating biological concepts and methods into research designed to address questions of interest to social and behavioral scientists (Harris 2010; (Weinstein et al., 2007). Less appreciated is the opportunity we have to colonize the biological sciences—as well as public discourse regarding the determinants of health—to have an impact on how we conceptualize and study human biology.

For the most part, research in the biological sciences privileges explanations “inside the body,” and is speeding down a reductionist road that elaborates cellular and molecular processes while ignoring contextual influences outside the body (Lewontin & Levins, 2007). As just one example, the sequencing of the human genome, accomplished in 2003, was celebrated as providing “… the first glimpse at our own instruction book,” and “… the possibility of achieving all we ever hoped for in medicine” (http://news.bbc.co.uk/2/hi/science/nature/807126.stm). Clinical medicine also privileges reduction, seeking to isolate single, proximate factors as causes of disease and as targets for treatment. Pathogens cause infection. Tumors cause cancer (Ahn, Tewari, Poon, & Phillips 2006).

In contrast, for more than 100 years, social scientists have documented the impact of contextual factors on human development, physiology, and health. For example, in the early 1900s, the anthropologist Franz Boas showed that cranial form—at the time interpreted as a fixed, inherited marker of racial identity—was in fact malleable, and that it changed within a single generation of immigrants to the US in response to environmental influences (Boas, 1912). For more than forty years, social scientists and social epidemiologists have reported that social relationships impact health, and that social isolation is a risk factor for early death that is comparable in magnitude to established risk factors such as smoking, obesity, and lack of physical activity (House, Landis, & Umberson, 1988). More recently, social isolation has been associated with physiological dysregulation in all stages of human development, pointing toward biological mechanisms through which social relationships affect health (Yang et al., 2016). Socioeconomic status—of keen interest to many social scientists—is consistently associated with multiple measures of physiological function, morbidity, and mortality (Adler et al., 1994;Yang et al. 2017; Wolfe, Evans & Seeman 2012).

Human biology is a social biology, and it is probably up to social scientists to make this point. Biosocial research, conducted in diverse, community-based settings, encourages an epistemological shift that reframes human biology, development, and health as complexly determined by multiple forces inside and outside the body. It engages issues and processes of interest to biological scientists, but foregrounds social/contextual factors as potentially important contributors to variation in human physiological function and health (Stinson, Bogin, & O’Rourke, 2012). This should be familiar ground for developmental and social/behavioral scientists who have long emphasized the complex interplay among genes, biology, and society across the life course (Engel, 1978; Glass & McAtee, 2006; Gottleib, 1991; Shanahan & Boardman, 2009). With an increasingly sophisticated toolkit for integrating biological measures into community-based, social science research, the time is right for a new generation of biosocial scholarship that enriches both the biological and the social sciences, and helps build stronger links between them.

Historically, community- and population-based research in the social sciences has relied on vital records or self-reported, survey-based measures of health and disease. Information can be readily collected from large representative samples, across a wide range of settings, but insight into biological processes is limited. In contrast, biomedical research employs in-depth biological measures collected in controlled clinical or laboratory settings, but typically relies on smaller, select groups of participants who are invited to participate based on pre-existing criteria. Generalizability and external validity are limited, and social factors are generally not considered, beyond standard measures of socioeconomic status or self-reported health behaviors.

Methodological options for collecting and generating biological data have expanded greatly over the past 15 years, allowing us to bridge this gap (Weinstein et al., 2007). Low cost, “field-friendly” options for collecting blood, saliva, or urine in the home or local community allow investigators to gain access to physiological information from large numbers of participants in naturalistic settings (Adam & Kumari, 2009; McDade, Williams, & Snodgrass, 2007). Developments in assay technology have facilitated the measurement of proteins, gene transcripts, epigenetic marks, and DNA sequences with higher resolution in smaller quantities of sample, at lower costs (Dedeurwaerder et al., 2011; McDade et al., 2016). Portable devices and low-cost monitors facilitate assessment of sleep, physical function and activity, blood pressure, and body size and composition (Lindau & McDade, 2007) (Marino et al, Sleep 2013).

These methodological innovations have encouraged wide scale integration of objective biological measures into social science surveys. For example, dried blood spots—drops of whole blood collected from a simple finger stick—have been collected from more than 35,000 participants in the US in studies like the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent to Adult Health (Add Health), the Health and Retirement Study, the National Social Life, Health, and Aging Project, and Moving to Opportunity. International studies, including the Cebu Longitudinal Health and Nutrition Survey, the Mexican Family Life Survey, and the Study on Global AGEing and Adult Health are collecting tens of thousands more. In another example, Add Health developed its own kit for the collection of buccal cell DNA in 1996 to test for the zygosity of sampled twin pairs. Ten years later, commercial kits for saliva DNA collection (e.g., Oragene) are routinely used by multiple studies to collect thousands of DNA samples both in the home setting and through the mail via self-collection.

The integration of objective measures of biological function and health has advanced the biosocial perspective by directly contributing to our understanding of how social, economic, and community factors shape human biology and health, and vice versa. These methods also address the goal of “socializing” biology: By taking our methods into the community, where participants are living their daily lives, we greatly expand the range of environmental variation that can be evaluated in relation to biological phenomena. Contextual factors are therefore brought into relief as potentially important determinants of human physiological function and health in ways not possible with lab- or clinic-based research designs. Lastly, these methods serve as a catalyst for productive collaboration among social, life, and biomedical scientists. The growing availability of social and biological data in large, representative samples, and the emphasis on interdisciplinary scholarship, has laid fertile ground for the integration of complementary expertise to generate novel insights into the ways in which social and biological processes interact in pathways of human development.

Human development is a process that has social and biological determinants and intergenerational linkages beginning in utero and continuing throughout all stages of the human life span (Hertzman & Boyce, 2010). Although there is a general consensus that early life conditions and childhood experiences matter for subsequent social and biological development in adolescence, early adulthood, mid adulthood and old age, most social and biomedical research does not capture the ways in which developmental processes are linked and interrelated across phases of human life, nor does it capture the dynamic interactions of social and biological forces that underlie development across time and space. Part of this research gap is due to a lack of longitudinal, multilevel life course data and intergenerational study designs, and part is due to disciplinary approaches designed to identify disciplinary-specific determinants of social, behavioral, or health outcomes at a point in time.

A life course perspective is essential in biosocial research because outcomes at any point in time reflect the product of prior interactions between social and biological forces that occur across human development (Shanahan, Hofer & Shanahan 2003). Life phases and social roles are often intimately tied to biological events or trajectories (George 2009). For example, a woman’s first birth marks her transition into parenthood just as menopause defines the end of the reproductive phase of her life. Although a woman can biologically become a mother when she reaches puberty in adolescence, most young people delay parenthood until well after puberty in the US to continue social and emotional maturation and invest in human capital and career development before becoming a parent. Thus, social and biological forces jointly shape transitions between roles and patterns of continuity and discontinuity that extend across the phases of life. Biosocial approaches, therefore, require the researcher to dynamically assess both biological and social features of the developing person and their changing social context through time and across generations to achieve a full understanding of the determinants of social and physical well-being.

Within social and behavioral sciences, research on aging has been at the forefront of biosocial approaches. Because aging is a process that integrates forces inside the body and outside the body to shape function and health in older adulthood (Figure 1), aging research has led the field in study designs incorporating inputs across social and biological levels of analysis. Understandably, this line of research focuses on phenomena such as disability, illness and disease, and longevity and mortality. The biosocial approach in aging research, however, has not been well-informed by a life course perspective.

For a long time, aging research used self-reported health and behavioral information and cross-sectional designs to study, for example, the age distribution of the prevalence of illness and chronic disease (e.g., National Center for Health Statistics 2016; Ward, Schiller & Goodman 2014); activities of daily living (ADLs) and instrumental activities of daily living (IADLs) designed to assess whether older adults can independently care for themselves (Freedman & Spillman 2014); and family and social relationships among the elderly (Waite & Das 2010). Demographic studies also use cross-sectional data but dynamic life table methodology to document onset and years of disability and chronic illness and to estimate mortality risks and life expectancy based on point-in-time rates of these respective events (Crimmins, Zhang & Saito 2016). Perhaps the most influential contribution of aging research with implications for the biosocial paradigm is longstanding evidence of large and persistent social gradients in health and mortality (Adler et al. 1994; Wolfe, Evans & Seeman 2012; Marmot & Wilkinson 2005). Still, this research remains primarily cross-sectional, documenting how SES is associated with aging-related outcomes at a point in time.

With the advent of nationally-representative longitudinal aging studies in the 1990s such as the Health and Retirement Study (HRS) and the National Social Life, Health and Aging Project (NSHAP), and longitudinal community-based aging studies such as Framingham, Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities Study (ARIC), and Reasons for Geographic and Racial Differences in Stroke (REGARDS), a life course design could now be applied to understand how previous social, behavioral, and environmental conditions were related to health and disease outcomes among older adults. In addition, as new survey field methods for measuring objective health outcomes (discussed above) were incorporated into many of these ongoing longitudinal aging studies, the ability to understand biological mechanisms and markers of health and disease further enhanced longitudinal life course data for biosocial research. However, the life phase examined in these longitudinal aging studies is still limited to older adult ages as these studies begin to observe individuals at ages 45 (e.g., ARIC) or 50 (e.g., HRS) or 55 (e.g., NSHAP), thus missing earlier life stages that certainly bear on aging processes which arguably begin at birth. The social gradient in health and mortality, for example, can now be studied by examining how SES trajectories beginning at age 50 influence the onset of disease or death in subsequent ages.

From a life course perspective, there are four major limitations to this research design. First, SES does not change much beyond age 50 because the components of socioeconomic status—education, occupation, income and wealth—are typically developed earlier in the life course during adolescence, early-, and mid-adulthood and vary little in old age. Second, the early- and mid-life biological precursors to disease onset and death are not observed in these studies. Relatedly, the lack of biological data prior to older ages precludes opportunities to consider how early life course health and biological processes shape SES attainments in adulthood. Fourth, studies that begin observation at older ages miss those who have died, typically the more disadvantaged individuals who experience greater exposure to earlier life trauma and illnesses and have fewer resources for health care, thus biasing the SES-health relationship. One solution to this lack of prior life course information that bears directly on older age social and physical well-being is to collect retrospective information about status in prior stages of life, enabling a modified life course perspective for biosocial research. Here aging studies have focused on retrospective reports of SES and health conditions at birth and during early childhood.

Along these lines, research on the developmental origins of health and disease (DOHaD) has exploded, following early biomedical research by Barker and colleagues documenting significant links between birth weight and later cardiovascular disease risk within cohorts (Barker 1997; 1998; 2006). The life course approach has had a major impact on epidemiologic research on the determinants of adult disease risk, with a particular emphasis on cardiovascular diseases and the physiological processes through which they are influenced by early life nutritional environments (Gluckman et al. 2008; Wadhwa et al. 2009, Smith & Ryckman 2015; Kuh & Ben-Shlomo 1997; 2004). Numerous studies have linked uterine, birth, and childhood exposures to adult physical health and disease (Bengtsson & Brostrom 2009; Cameron & Demerath 2002; Crimmins & Finch 2006; Smith & Ryckman 2015). Demographic and social research on the “long arm of childhood” has also demonstrated the value of a life course perspective where early life circumstances are both directly and indirectly associated with health outcomes that emerge decades later in adulthood (Blackwell et al. 2001; Case & Paxson 2010; Elo & Preston 1992; Hayward & Gorman 2004; Preston et al. 1998).

Most of this research, however, links early life conditions with physiological processes or chronic disease outcomes in later adulthood with cross-sectional research designs, with limited attention to what happens in between—during the majority of the early life course from later childhood to adulthood. From a biosocial perspective, this means we are missing a lot, especially the social processes and contexts that structure, mediate, and moderate biology over the life course.

Adolescence and the transition to adulthood, for example, are life stages when young people first begin to choose their environments, health behaviors, habits and future lifestyles (Harris 2010). These life course choices shape or alter social and biological pathways originating in childhood and moving into adulthood. Investments in human capital begin in early childhood, but intensify and become more self-directed during adolescence and the transition to adulthood. Profound and protracted physical, biological, and neurological changes linked to puberty occur throughout adolescence and early adulthood. Hormonal changes prompt a literal remodeling of cortical and limbic circuits in the brain that were previously organized in the perinatal period and that, in combination with adolescent social experiences and contexts, affect general cognition, decision making, and behavior into adulthood (Sisk & Zehr 2005). Behavioral changes and exploration in diet, exercise, sleep patterns, substance use, sexual activity, and aggression during adolescence and young adulthood further shape social and biological pathways into adulthood (Hubert et al. 1987). DOHaD research, and other life course perspectives, ideally should include the contributions of these critical developmental stages to illuminate health and well-being pathways into adulthood.

While neurological development and change slows down as young people settle into adulthood, these demographically dense years bring new stresses to daily life as young adults juggle the multiple interrelated life domains of relationships, schooling, work, and family. Stress processes are perhaps the most commonly considered biological mechanisms through which the social environment gets under the skin to affect health and development in biosocial models, as described earlier in the case of the stress hormone cortisol (Gruenewald 2004; McEwen 2002). Middle adulthood may usher in greater life course stability and security in socioeconomic status, work, and family, but only for certain subgroups of the population. Middle adulthood has become more dynamic and demanding in contemporary US society with high divorce and re-partnering rates, greater dependency from both the child and parent generations, and uncertain work schedules, low wages, and a lack of employment benefits for those with low education or few job skills. Middle-aged adults are highly embedded in social relationships compared to other life stages with aging parents, children, the parents of children’s friends, neighbors, work colleagues and within community institutions. These relationships are important social mechanisms that can buffer (e.g., through social support) or exacerbate (e.g., through strain and conflict) the daily stresses of middle adulthood (Yang, Schorpp & Harris 2014; Yang et al. 2016).

All life course stages have unique social and biological forces that determine life-long human development and that operate independently and jointly to influence physical and social well-being in that life stage and beyond. Biosocial research cannot examine social and biological forces in all life stages in one project or with one dataset, but should contribute knowledge about how social and biological phenomena operate in distinct life stages and are linked to health and social inequities in subsequent stages across the life course. While we have made some progress documenting the association between early life conditions and late life health and disease outcomes (e.g., lower birth weight and increased CVD risk), we need to move beyond cross-sectional designs to uncover the underlying life course processes that explain these associations. Both the inter-generational precursors that lead to these conditions and the subsequent intra-generational life course pathways such conditions initiate are yet to be explored. There is, however, a small, but growing, literature that links these kinds of early life health factors to later social attainments, but more research on health and biology as underlying factors in social stratification processes is needed.

Here we describe two general life course orientations for understanding how biological phenomena are related to social and economic status and opportunities in direct, indirect, and reciprocal ways. One orientation examines how social stratification processes across the life course are related to subsequent health outcomes in different life stages. This orientation stems primarily from the large literature on the social gradients of health discussed above (or more commonly, the social determinants of health), but with a life course perspective. Social stratification is both an inter- and intra-generational process. At birth we enter a social hierarchy tied to parental SES that determines access to material and social resources for both physical and social development. The developing individual then faces constraints and opportunities in each life stage that determine her social and economic status across time. Inter- and intra-generational social stratification processes have both direct and indirect effects on health across the life course. Understanding the social and biological mechanisms for how social stratification processes get under the skin to influence health is at the heart of this conceptual orientation of biosocial research.

Ideally, social stratification is measured longitudinally, as a life course process, conceived as “social exposures” that can be positive (e.g., supportive parenting behaviors, college education) or negative (e.g., childhood poverty, neighborhood disadvantage). Exposure to both beneficial and adverse experiences over the life course will vary for each individual and constitute a unique social stratification trajectory. Biological outcomes are conceived of as the consequence of exposure in social stratification trajectories and can be measured at a point in time in a particular life stage, or over time as biological and developmental change. Importantly, social stratification trajectories represent social processes that enable the biosocial researcher to explore fundamental life course mechanisms involving the timing, duration, and intensity of beneficial or adverse social exposures that occur in different and across phases of life and that affect health and development in subsequent life stages. The ability to measure the timing, duration, and intensity of social exposures across the life course allows for testing life course models for how social experiences that occur outside the body are linked to biological mechanisms inside the body that affect health and well-being.

The stress response framework is the most prominent biosocial paradigm that explicates how trajectories of social structural inequalities are associated with greater exposure to stress and its biological and health-related manifestations (Pearlin 1989; Aneshensel 1992; McEwen 1998; 2002). When social exposures are intense, or the magnitude of structural disadvantage is high (e.g., the depth of poverty, multiple disadvantages of poor neighborhoods), stress response is chronic and biological dysregulation is greater, resulting in poor health and developmental outcomes. The life stage timing of social exposures, however, may differ for both the biological mechanisms and subsequent health outcomes associated with stress exposure. Figure 2 provides an illustration of various life course models that describe how exposure to social disadvantage in particular developmental periods may operate to increase health risk in subsequent life stages.

The top model illustrates “sensitive period” timing effects in which exposures during sensitive periods of development have stronger effects on health outcomes than they would at other life stages (Hayward & Gorman 2004; Gluckman et al. 2008; Cohen et al. 2010). Sensitive period effects operate through a “biological embedding” mechanism whereby social exposures during sensitive windows of development have the potential to induce structural and functional changes to the developing individual through biological programming that cannot be reversed irrespective of intervening experience. Thus, the dark shadowed line represents a direct effect of exposure in the earlier stage of development with no indirect effects and no direct effects of subsequent social disadvantage on later life health. This life course model posits that the effect of the sensitive period exposure is typically “latent” in that its impact on health outcomes may not appear until later life stages, often decades later.

Duration effects of social stratification processes can be explored through the accumulation life course model (middle model of Figure 2), which emphasizes the role of persistent advantage or disadvantage over time—in both specific life stages and over life stages—on health and development. The effects of multiple exposures over the life course are both additive and interactive and combine in synergistic ways to influence biological mechanisms and, in turn, health and development outcomes. Cumulative effects can either be multiple exposures to a recurrent stressor (e.g., chronic poverty) or a series of exposures to different social environments or life experiences. For example, poverty experienced only during childhood is not as detrimental as poverty during childhood, adolescence, and the transition to adulthood on subsequent adult health.

A third life course model that might explain how social stratification processes are related to health outcomes is the pathway model which tracks how social exposures in one life stage influence the probability of related social exposures in subsequent life stages. Also known as the “chains of risk” model, it emphasizes pathway effects whereby early experiences set in motion a chain of events that put individuals on paths differentiated by types and levels of stress exposures to social and biological factors (Marmot et al. 2001; Pudrovska & Anikputa 2014). This model elaborates on the ways in which inter- and intra-generational social stratification pathways are linked across the life course. For example, the connection between early life conditions and adult health and disease may be explained by the SES pathway where early life SES determines adult SES, which in turn, is a more proximate and important predictor of adult health and disease (Yang et al. 2017).

A second orientation for understanding biosocial pathways in well-being across the life course is consideration of the role of biology and/or health in social stratification processes (Palloni 2006). In this orientation, biological mechanisms and health trajectories are important contributors to subsequent socioeconomic outcomes and attainment. For example, economic research has focused on how childhood health influences human capital and labor force outcomes in adulthood, including educational attainment, labor force participation, income and occupation (Case, Lubotsky & Paxson 2002; Currie & Stabile 2003; Currie & Moretti 2007; Case, Fertig & Paxson 2005; Almond & Currie 2011). Figure 3 illustrates the case of how early life course health can influence later socioeconomic status. In particular, life course trajectories of obesity during adolescence and into young adulthood contribute to social stratification outcomes in adulthood. Research findings indicate that those who become obese in early adolescence and remain obese for a longer period of time have lower levels of education, marriage, wages, household income, employment, assets, and subjective social status (Cawley 2004; Han, Norton & Stearns 2009; Harris & Lee 2011; Glass, Haas & Reither 2010). These life course effects of obesity operate through such mechanisms as low self-esteem, social isolation, societal views of attractiveness and lost work days, illustrating the biosocial connections. Similar effects are found for chronic health conditions and diabetes during adolescence and young adulthood which truncate educational trajectories and reduce the stability of work (Fletcher 2012).

In this orientation life course models will illuminate the often missing, underlying role of biology in social stratification processes. Understanding whether, when, and how biological processes matter for social and economic outcomes across the life course will help to identify when biomedical interventions might be most effective for reducing social inequality. These life course models are not mutually exclusive and in reality coexist (see Hallqvist et al. 2004). Most importantly, they provide a framework for biosocial research made possible by longitudinal data and study designs that enable researchers to identify the social and biological processes that operate in pathways of well-being across the life span. The life course perspective articulates the longitudinal and multidimensional of social and biological forces that operate in all life stages and underlie human development across time, emphasizing the need to conceptualize social conditions and biological mechanisms as dynamic constructs that unfold across time, beginning in early life and continuing into young adulthood, midlife, and old age.

There is now widespread consensus that social, behavioral, and health outcomes are a function of both nature and nurture, and are best understood in a life course context. Even in the age of genome mapping, research on the impact of genetic variance alone has limited explanatory power, and is often of less interest to social and behavioral scientists given that individual DNA sequence is fixed and not subject to intervention. Instead, social and behavioral scientists have been drawn to understanding gene-environment interplay, or how environmental and genetic factors interact over time to affect social, behavioral, and health outcomes, along the lines of Figure 1. Because such outcomes represent the cumulative history of a person’s social experiences as they combine with his or her genetic makeup, gene-environment interplay reflects life course processes. While there is widespread appreciation that the links between genes and behavioral outcomes, for example, are conditioned by the social environment, there is less consideration of the dynamic features of social environments and life experiences as processes occurring across the life course. To date, most studies of gene-environment interplay are cross-sectional or use longitudinal data without explicit modeling of life course features including pathways, transitions, trajectories, durations, or timing (Conley 2016; Shanahan & Boardman 2009).

Nevertheless, there is substantial social science research examining gene-environment interplay focusing on two general approaches: gene-environment interactions (GxE) and gene-environment correlations (rGE). GxE research has captured social science attention by elaborating on processes by which the effect of genetic factors on a social or biological outcome is conditioned by environmental factors and vice versa (Boardman, Daw & Freese 2013; Hutter et al. 2013). Provocative findings have been published, for example, showing that genetic effects on children’s cognition are dampened in low SES environments, genetic propensities for adolescent substance use are enhanced or suppressed according to the prevalence of substance use in the adolescents’ schools, stressful life events increase the risk of depression depending on one’s genetic profile for processing neurotransmitters, and adolescents are more genetically similar to their friends in more highly structured and segregated environments (Boardman et al. 2012a; Caspi et al. 2003; Rowe 1999; Daw et al. 2013). While such findings highlight the important role of the social environment in genetic processes, especially from a policy perspective, GxE research has come under significant criticism for a poor record of replication, lack of statistical power for GxE associations, and the endogenous nature of most measures of E (Boardman, Daw & Freese. 2013; Conley 2016; Freese 2009; Charney & English 2012; North & Martin 2008). Still, promising GxE research is on the horizon using natural or quasi-experimental designs and larger samples afforded through genetic consortia (e.g., Boardman et al. 2012b; Okbay et al. 2016; Schmitz & Conley 2015, 2016; Rietveld et al. 2013).

Research exploring gene-environment correlations is especially valuable to social and behavioral sciences as this line of research confronts the worrisome endogeneity problem of estimated environmental effects being due to unobserved heterogeneity (e.g., genetic factors) associated with the selection of one’s environment. rGE refers to processes by which genetic factors are associated with features of the environments in which individuals live their lives (e.g., friendships, peer groups, romantic relationships, schools, neighborhoods, work environments, etc.). Sorting out and controlling for genetic variance in selection of these environments enables social and behavioral scientists to isolate the causal impacts of social environments on social, behavioral, and health outcomes. In sum, perhaps the main impact of the evidence on gene-environment interplay has been to dispel notions of the nature-nurture dichotomy and build consensus on the need for integrative models of genetic and social factors to better understand human development and health. But two general weaknesses remain: the G (i.e., genotype) is still fixed and therefore unresponsive to social change; and the dynamics of life course changes and their biological interactions with changing social environments and experiences over time have not been exploited in either GxE or rGE research.

Human social genomics, on the other hand, is an emerging field of research that examines why and how external social conditions affect the activity of the genome (Slavich & Cole 2013; Boyce & Kobor 2015). Social genomics includes the study of gene expression (transcriptome) and epigenetics (epigenome) and it emerges from the scientific understanding that while the gene sequences we inherit from our parents are fixed, the expression of these genes is shaped by forces “outside” the body. The focus of social genomics is inherently biosocial as it seeks to uncover how social experiences can alter gene expression and thereby affect physiological function and social and behavioral outcomes (Cole 2014; Hertzman 2012). Indeed, human social genomics research is demonstrating that certain genes can be “turned on” or “turned off” by different social-environmental conditions, and in some cases these social exposures can affect the activity of hundreds of genes in a coordinated manner.

This line of research provides new opportunities for understanding how social and genetic factors interact to shape complex biological and social pathways of well-being. Indeed, it has the potential to reframe our understanding of the genome as a dynamic substrate that incorporates information from the environment over developmental time, rather than the prevailing view of the genome as static sequences of DNA that are fixed at conception. Current social genomics research has examined, for example, how social processes, such as social status, social supports and/or isolation, social capital, early life adversity, exposure to toxicants and microbes, and health behaviors, alter the expression of hundreds of human genes (e.g., suppression of antiviral and antibody-related genes and stimulation of pro-inflammatory genes) to impact human development and health over many years (e.g., Cole 2013; 2014; Murphy et al. 2012; Miller et al. 2009; Fry et al. 2012; McDade et al., in press).

The term epigenetics—first used by Waddington in the 1940s (Jablonka & Lamb 2002)—literally means “above” or “on top of” genetics, and refers to chemical modifications to DNA and its packaging that change the accessibility of gene regions to transcription factors, and thereby affect the level of transcription. These modifications alter the physical structure of DNA in ways that are relatively stable and conserved with cell replication. Therefore, epigenetics represents a biological mechanism through which the body “remembers” prior environmental exposures to shape gene expression—a key reason why epigenetics has captured the attention of many social scientists. Methylation of DNA has been the major focus of human research, and involves the binding of methyl groups to cytosine residues in CpG dinucleotides (Bird 2002). At a point in time in an individual’s life course, analysis of DNA methylation may reveal how and which genes have been modified in response to the cumulative life course environmental, behavioral, and biological trajectories of that individual. Thus, epigenetics specifies a life course biosocial process that entails the dynamic interactions and feedback loops of social and genetic phenomena both inter- and intra-generationally over the life course (Boyce & Kobor 2015). For example, epigenetic patterns have been shown to be altered by a range of environmental conditions such as diet, tobacco smoking, exercise, and exposure to chemicals (Christensen et al. 2009; Gronniger et al. 2010; Langevin et al. 2011). More broadly, measures of socioeconomic and psychosocial adversity in childhood have been linked to patterns of DNA methylation later in life (Essex et al. 2013; Needham et al. 2015). Epigenetic patterns have also been shown to affect physical traits and appearance, behavior, and health outcomes (IHEC 2013). Thus, environmental variation may routinely change epigenetic patterns, and those epigenetic patterns may, in turn, influence developmental outcomes over time.

One of the more provocative—but also controversial—findings for social scientists is that environmentally-triggered behavior or biological change might be transmitted across generations through epigenetic mechanisms and without the involvement of DNA sequence (Jablonka & Lamb, 2015; Thayer & Kuzawa, 2011). Fascinating early evidence on the inheritance of epigenetic marks comes from research done on mice, while human evidence for intergenerational epigenetic inheritance is much more difficult to establish. Studies of humans whose ancestors survived through periods of starvation in Sweden and the Netherlands suggest that the effects of famine on epigenetics and development can pass through at least three generations (Heijmans et al. 2008; Tobi et al. 2009). Nutrient deprivation in a recent ancestor seems to prime the body for diabetes and cardiovascular problems, a biological response that may have evolved to mitigate the effects of future famines. The findings on intergenerational epigenetic inheritance could have far-reaching significance. Much social science research documents how parental characteristics, such as lifestyle, behaviors, and living habits, influence children’s well-being. Rather than this process occurring through social mechanisms, an epigenetic mechanism suggests the possibility that lifestyles and behaviors may be converted into epigenetic marks, stored and transmitted to children and their children’s children who do not have any direct environmental exposure to these lifestyles or behaviors.

Social genomics is an exciting area for future biosocial research that emphasizes the instrumental role of the social environment in altering how genes are expressed to affect behavior, biology, and social and health outcomes. The more we understand how the social environment regulates genes that impact health and social stratification processes, the more potential we have for intervening on those environmental exposures to reduce health and social inequalities. The molecular models of social genomics do require new methodological skills and technical capacities for working with these data, over and above the application of standard social science methods used in GxE and rGE analysis. Interdisciplinary training, however, is already coming online to equip social scientists with these skills through summer boot camps and graduate training programs around the country. The potential for understanding these social and biological phenomena has captured the attention of the scholarly and public worlds alike. The ability of social genomics to fill diverse gaps in our understanding of human development and health and to provide scientific explanations of the mechanisms underlying our lived experiences makes it a very compelling avenue for future biosocial research.

Contributors to the volume represent a wide range of disciplines, and their work advances the biosocial perspective along many of the lines described above. The special issue is loosely organized around three themes, and here we briefly outline these themes and the chapters within them.

The impact of social adversity on human welfare is of longstanding concern to social scientists. A biosocial perspective addresses questions regarding the health impacts of adversity, and the biological mechanisms through which social environments “get under the skin” to impact human development and health. This aspect of biosocial research has been greatly advanced by recent methodological developments which have facilitated the collection of objective biological data in non-clinical, community- and population-based settings. All three chapters in this section showcase the value of these kinds of measures for advancing our understanding of how social adversity impacts health.

Massey and colleagues build on a longstanding tradition of scholarship on neighborhood effects, with a particular emphasis on residential segregation and concentrated poverty. The majority of this work has been sociological, with some links to health but very little attention to biology. Massey et al. introduce a biosocial framework for linking spatially concentrated disadvantage, at the geographic level, with an individual-level biological measure of cellular aging (telomere length) to reveal mechanisms through which social structure contributes to race-based differences in morbidity and mortality in the US. Aside from its contribution to the literature on neighborhood effects, the chapter demonstrates how collaborative, interdisciplinary teams can leverage novel insights from molecular biology to cast new light on longstanding social science questions.

The chapter by Goosby and colleagues also investigates the health impact of social disadvantage, but at the individual, micro-social level of analysis. Their focus on perceived discrimination draws on a well-established line of biosocial research that attends to the appraisal of stress as a key part of the causal pathway linking social adversity with physical health. Sleep quality and quantity are the key outcomes in their study, based on recent clinical and epidemiological research demonstrating the importance of sleep for a wide range of physical and mental health outcomes. As with Massey et al., the chapter showcases the value of borrowing from biological and health sciences to illuminate issues of interest to social scientists. This point is underscored by the counter-intuitive nature of their results: Global ratings of discrimination are negatively associated with sleep quantity and quality, as one might expect, but participants slept better the night following a day when they reported a discriminatory encounter. This finding reveals the potential of biological or health-related measures to provide novel insights into psychosocial dynamics that might otherwise be obscured. It also highlights the value of measuring these dynamics at multiple levels of analysis.

The chapter by McClure and colleagues provides an excellent example of how a biosocial approach can be utilized to “socialize” biology. The ratio of signal-joint to beta T-cell receptor excision circles (sj/beta-TREC ratio) is used by immunologists to measure the function of the thymus, an organ that produces cells (T lymphocytes) that are essential for immunity. In their study in Detroit following the Great Recession, they show that neighborhood-level measures of home foreclosure and abandonment predict lower levels of thymic function among residents. Furthermore, reduced social cohesion—a product of home foreclosure and abandonment—predicts lower thymic function. With a clearly articulated conceptual model that informs their study design and analytic strategy, McClure et al. move from the macro- (Great Recession) to the mezzo- (neighborhood characteristics) to the micro- (thymic function) levels of analysis to demonstrate how human biology is a social biology.

Human development is a process that has social and biological determinants and intergenerational linkages beginning in utero and continuing throughout all stages of the human life span. Two chapters in this section use a biosocial approach to assess the biological and social features of the developing person and their changing social contexts through time and space to provide new insights into the determinants of social and physical well-being. Research in these chapters examines differential outcomes in specific life stages as a function of earlier life course exposures and interactions between social and biological forces that occur across human development.

Qu and colleagues use a biopsychosocial approach to understand adolescent development among a growing ethnic minority group in the U.S.—Mexican American youth. They identify adolescence as a dynamic life stage in which neural changes in both brain function and brain structure are likely associated with individual differences in academic and psychological adjustment. They also argue that the environment becomes especially salient during adolescence for Mexican American youth when ethnic parents attempt to socialize children about their cultural values and heritage, and at the same time, adolescents yearn to spend more time with peers and fit into adolescent social life and activities. The longitudinal research in this chapter examines the independent and interactive effects of adolescents’ brain development and their family and peer environment in determining educational achievement and substance use. Through the integration of imaging data with rich social variables on cultural socialization and peer deviance, Qu and colleagues find important independent contributions of biological and psychosocial factors in youth’s achievement and adjustment.

The Jackson and Short chapter uses a life course intragenerational design to examine gender differences in physical health in young adulthood, and the ways in which adolescent development and social environments might explain the gender differentials. Documenting gender differences in objective biological markers of health (inflammation and immunosuppression) during young adulthood is a contribution to the health disparities literature given most research on sex differences focuses on older aged populations. Jackson and Short report strong differentials in inflammation and immune function that disadvantage women in these biological systems. They explore a wealth of childhood, adolescent, and early adulthood circumstances—including demographic, family socioeconomic, health behavior, and young adult family formation and socioeconomic attainment—as potential explanatory factors underlying the sex differences. Identifying gender disparities early in the adult life course is critical to curbing their growth throughout adulthood by designing interventions to improve female health and reduce the disparities before chronic disease and long term physical damage occurs.

Interest in gene-environment interplay has captured the imagination of biosocial researchers by isolating the role of genes in relation to environmental influence and focusing on the ways in which genes and environments operate together in social stratification processes across the life course. The advent of new sources of molecular genetic data, especially genome-wide data, and statistical tools for analyzing massive amounts of individual-level genome-wide data linked to survey and biomarker information in large studies has opened up exciting new research opportunities for understanding gene-environment interplay in biosocial models of attainment and behavior. Two chapters in this special issue take advantage of an analytic approach that combines the genetic associations with specific phenotypes (i.e., behavioral, attainment, and health outcomes) across the entire genome using polygenic scores (PGS) based on genome-wide association studies (GWAS). A PGS is a linear combination of the effects of genetic variants present in the entire genome specific to a phenotype that can be interpreted as a single quantitative measure of genetic predisposition for that phenotype.

The chapter by Mills and colleagues builds upon their recently published meta-GWAS study on human reproductive behavior (Barban et al. 2016) by examining the predictive power of two PGS discovered in that study for age at first birth and number of children ever born in four independent extant data sources. While there has been extensive research on the role of genetics in such outcomes as obesity, substance use, and education, they note little attention has been directed to fertility behavior perhaps because age at first birth and number of children ever born are complex outcomes related to biological fecundity, behavioral choice, and socio-environmental factors. Consistent with most of the research on genetic influence using molecular data, they find relatively low levels of predictive power for the PGSs based on the entire genome, revealing the more predominant role of social environmental and behavioral factors in determining age at first birth and number of children ever born. Rather than speculating on how much environmental and behavioral estimates are overstated by their confounding with genetic effects, these models enable social scientists to control for this genetic confounding while estimating the importance of social and behavioral factors of fertility behavior. Moreover, age at first birth and number of children ever born are well-established markers of social stratification, illustrated by the voluminous literature on teenage childbearing and family size (reviews in Furstenberg 2003; Powell et al. 2016; Sweeney and Raley 2014; Wilcox and Lerman 2014). Indeed, Mills and her coauthors show that the PGSs are also correlated with other fertility traits, such as childlessness, and are independent of the effects of education.

Domingue and colleagues focus on the role of genes in environmental selection processes, or gene-environment correlation. They examine geographic clustering of PGSs for multiple phenotypes related to anthropometry, education, and physical and mental health by state of residence at different points in the life course to explore the extent to which state-level genetic composition explains state-level clustering of various phenotypes and how these relationships change over age. Domingue and his coauthors expertly discuss the important motivation behind examining gene-environment correlations, the mechanisms through which gene-environment correlations may operate, and test for the penetrance of PGSs (association of genotype and phenotype) at both the individual and ecological (i.e., state) levels. For most of the phenotypes they examine, they find that the ecological correlations are much larger than the individual correlations, suggesting the environmental context of the state may moderate the genotype-phenotype associations. In particular, they identify two phenotypes, depression and educational attainment, for which the genetic context of a state is especially salient.

The chapters in this volume advance our understanding of the biosocial pathways of well-being across the life course, and their complex associations with social stratification. They build on a solid foundation of biosocial research in the social sciences, and they showcase the value of blurring the boundaries between phenomena outside the body and inside the body. In some cases they are using novel methods to cast new light on old questions. In others, novel methods are reframing the questions, and opening up new lines of inquiry. In all cases, the integration of biological information with measures of social environments and behavior across the life course is generating unique insights and unprecedented opportunities for discovery. In many ways, this volume can be seen as marking the “coming of age” of a new generation of biosocial scholarship, and the future looks bright for those of us who are invested in illuminating the complex pathways linking society, biology, and health across the life course.

  • Adam EK, & Kumari M (2009). Assessing salivary cortisol in large-scale, epidemiological research. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34(10), 1423–1436. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.06.011 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Adler NE, Boyce T, Chesney MA, Cohen S, Folkman S, Kahn RL, & Syme SL (1994). Socioeconomic status and health: The challenge of the gradient. American Psychologist, 49(1), 15–24. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Ahn AC, Tewari M, Poon CS, & Phillips RS (2006). The limits of reductionism in medicine: could systems biology offer an alternative? PLoS Med, 3(6), e208. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.0030208 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Almond D (2006). Is the 1918 Influenza Pandemic Over? Long-Term Effects of In Utero Influenza Exposure in the Post-1940 U.S. Population. Journal of Political Economy, 114(4), 672–712. doi: 10.1086/507154 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Almond D, & Currie J (2011). Killing Me Softly: The Fetal Origins Hypothesis. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 25(3), 153–172. doi: 10.1257/jep.25.3.153 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Aneshensel C (1992). Social Stress: Theory and Research. Annual Review of Sociology, 18(1), 15–38. doi: 10.1146/annurev.soc.18.1.15 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Anne C, & Christina P (2010). Causes and Consequences of Early-Life Health. Demography, 47(S), S65–S85. doi: 10.1353/dem.2010.0007 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Barban N, Jensen R, de Vlaming R, Vaez A, Mandemakers JJ, & Tropf FC (2016). Genome-wide analysis identifies 12 loci influencing human reproductive behavior. Nature Genetics. doi: doi: 10.1038/ng.3698 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Barker DJP (1997). Maternal nutrition, fetal nutrition, and disease in later life. Nutrition, 13(9), 807–813. doi: 10.1016/s0899-9007(97)00193-7 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Barker DJP (1998). Mothers, babies and health in later life (2nd ed.). Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone. [Google Scholar]
  • Barker DJP (2006). Adult Consequences of Fetal Growth Restriction. Clinical Obstetrics and Gynecology, 49(2), 270–283. doi: 10.1097/00003081-200606000-00009 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Ben-Shlomo Y, & Kuh D (2002). A life course approach to chronic disease epidemiology: conceptual models, empirical challenges and interdisciplinary perspectives. International Journal of Epidemiology, 31(2), 285–293. doi: 10.1093/ije/31.2.285 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Bengtsson T, & Broström G (2009). Do conditions in early life affect old-age mortality directly and indirectly? Evidence from 19th-century rural Sweden. Social Science & Medicine, 68(9), 1583–1590. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2009.02.020 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Bird A (2002). DNA methylation patterns and epigenetic memory. Genes & Development 16, 6–21. doi: 10.1101/gad.947102 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Blackwell DL, Hayward MD, & Crimmins EM (2001). Does childhood health affect chronic morbidity in later life? Social Science & Medicine, 52(8), 1269–1284. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(00)00230-6 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Boardman JD, Daw J, & Freese J (2013). Defining the Environment in Gene–Environment Research: Lessons From Social Epidemiology. American Journal of Public Health, 103(S1), S64–S72. doi: 10.2105/ajph.2013.301355 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Boardman JD, Domingue BW, & Daw J (2015). What can genes tell us about the relationship between education and health? Social Science & Medicine, 127, 171–180. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.08.001 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Boardman JD, Domingue BW, & Fletcher JM (2012a). How social and genetic factors predict friendship networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(43), 17377–17381. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1208975109 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Boardman JD, Roettger ME, Domingue BW, McQueen MB, Haberstick BC, & Harris KM (2012b). Gene-environment interactions related to body mass: School policies and social context as environmental moderators. Journal of Theoretical Politics, 24(3), 370–388. doi: 10.1177/0951629812437751 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Boas F (1912). Changes in the Bodily Form of Descendants of Immigrants. American Anthropologist, 14(3), 530–562. doi: DOI 10.1525/aa.1912.14.3.02a00080 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Cameron N, & Demerath EW (2002). Critical periods in human growth and their relationship to diseases of aging. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 119(S35), 159–184. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.10183 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Case A, & Paxson C (2010). Causes and consequences of early-life health. Demography 47(Suppl 1), S65–S85. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Case A, Fertig A, & Paxson C (2005). The lasting impact of childhood health and circumstance. Journal of Health Economics, 24(2), 365–389. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2004.09.008 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Case A, Lubotsky D, & Paxson C (2002). Economic Status and Health in Childhood: The Origins of the Gradient. American Economic Review, 92(5), 1308–1334. doi: 10.1257/000282802762024520 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Caspi A (2003). Influence of Life Stress on Depression: Moderation by a Polymorphism in the 5- HTT Gene. Science, 301(5631), 386–389. doi: 10.1126/science.1083968 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Cawley J (2004). The impact of obesity on wages. The Journal of Human Resources 39(2), 451–474. [Google Scholar]
  • Charney E, & English W (2012). Candidate Genes and Political Behavior. American Political Science Review, 106(01), 1–34. doi: 10.1017/s0003055411000554 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Christensen BC, Houseman EA, Marsit CJ, Zheng S, Wrensch MR, Wiemels JL, … Kelsey KT (2009). Aging and Environmental Exposures Alter Tissue-Specific DNA Methylation Dependent upon CpG Island Context. PLoS Genetics, 5(8), e1000602. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1000602 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Cohen S, Schwartz JE, Epel E, Kirschbaum C, Sidney S, & Seeman T (2006). Socioeconomic status, race, and diurnal cortisol decline in the Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults (CARDIA) Study. Psychosom Med, 68(1), 41–50. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000195967.51768.ea [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Cohen S, Janicki-Deverts D, Chen D, & Matthews KA (2010). Childhood socioeconomic status and adult health. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1186, 37–55. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Cole SW (2013). Social Regulation of Human Gene Expression: Mechanisms and Implications for Public Health. American Journal of Public Health, 103(S1), S84–S92. doi: 10.2105/ajph.2012.301183 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Cole SW (2014). Human Social Genomics. PLoS Genetics, 10(8), e1004601. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1004601 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Conley D (2016). Socio-Genomic Research Using Genome-Wide Molecular Data. Annual Review of Sociology, 42(1), 275–299. doi: 10.1146/annurev-soc-081715-074316 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Conley D, & Bennett NG (2000). Is biology destiny? Birth weight and life chances. American Sociological Review, 65(3), 458–467. doi: Doi 10.2307/2657467 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Crimmins EM, & Finch CE (2005). Infection, inflammation, height, and longevity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103(2), 498–503. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0501470103 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Crimmins EM, Zhang Y, & Saito Y (2016). Trends Over 4 Decades in Disability-Free Life Expectancy in the United States. American Journal of Public Health, 106(7), 1287–1293. doi: 10.2105/ajph.2016.303120 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Currie J, & Moretti E (2007). Biology as Destiny? Short- and Long-Run Determinants of Intergenerational Transmission of Birth Weight. Journal of Labor Economics, 25(2), 231–264. doi: 10.1086/511377 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Currie J, & Stabile M (2003). Socioeconomic Status and Child Health: Why Is the Relationship Stronger for Older Children? American Economic Review, 93(5), 1813–1823. doi: 10.1257/000282803322655563 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Daw J, Shanahan M, Harris KM, Smolen A, Haberstick B, & Boardman JD (2013). Genetic Sensitivity to Peer Behaviors: 5HTTLPR, Smoking, and Alcohol Consumption. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 54(1), 92–108. doi: 10.1177/0022146512468591 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Dedeurwaerder S, Defrance M, Calonne E, Denis H, Sotiriou C, & Fuks F (2011). Evaluation of the Infinium Methylation 450K technology. Epigenomics, 3(6), 771–784. doi: Doi 10.2217/Epi.11.105 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • DeSantis AS, Adam EK, Doane LD, Mineka S, Zinbarg RE, & Craske MG (2007). Racial/ethnic differences in cortisol diurnal rhythms in a community sample of adolescents. J Adolesc Health, 41(1), 3–13. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2007.03.006 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Elo IT, & Preston SH (1992). Effects of Early-Life Conditions on Adult Mortality: A Review. Population Index, 58(2), 186. doi: 10.2307/3644718 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Engel GL (1978). The biopsychosocial model and the education of health professionals. Ann N Y Acad Sci, 310, 169–187. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Essex MJ, Boyce WT, Hertzman C, Lam LL, Armstrong JM, Neumann SM, & Kobor MS (2013). Epigenetic vestiges of early developmental adversity: childhood stress exposure and DNA methylation in adolescence. Child Dev, 84(1), 58–75. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01641.x [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Fletcher JM, & Richards MR (2012). Diabetes’s ‘health shock’ to schooling and earning: Increased dropout rates and lower wages and employment in young adults. Health Affairs 31(1), 27–34. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.2011.0862 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Freedman VA, & Spillman BC (2014). Disability and Care Needs Among Older Americans. Milbank Quarterly, 92(3), 509–541. doi: 10.1111/1468-0009.12076 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Freese J, & Shostak S (2009). Genetics and Social Inquiry. Annual Review of Sociology, 35(1), 107–128. doi: 10.1146/annurev-soc-070308-120040 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Fry RC, Rager JE, Zhou H, Zou B, Brickey JW, Ting J, … Alexis NE (2012). Individuals with increased inflammatory response to ozone demonstrate muted signaling of immune cell trafficking pathways. Respiratory Research, 13(1), 89. doi: 10.1186/1465-9921-13-89 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Furstenberg FF (2003). Teenage Childbearing as a Public Issue and Private Concern. Annual Review of Sociology, 29(1), 23–39. doi: 10.1146/annurev.soc.29.010202.100205 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • George LK (2009). Conceptualizing and Measuring Trajectories. In Elder GH & Giele JZ (Eds.), The Craft of Life Course Research (pp. 163–186). New York: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
  • Glass C, Haas S, & Reither E (2010). The skinny on success: Adolescent body mass, gender and occupational attainment. Social Forces 88(4), 1777–1806. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Glass TA, & McAtee MJ (2006). Behavioral science at the crossroads in public health: Extending horizons, envisioning the future. Social Science & Medicine, 62(7), 1650–1671. doi: DOI 10.1016/j.soescimed.2005.08.044 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Gluckman PD, Hanson MA, Cooper C, & Thornburg KL (2008). Effect of In Utero and Early-Life Conditions on Adult Health and Disease. New England Journal of Medicine, 359(1), 61–73. doi: 10.1056/nejmra0708473 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Gottleib G (1991). Experiential canalization of behavioral devlopment: theory. Developmental Psychology, 27, 4–13. [Google Scholar]
  • Gravlee CC (2009). How Race Becomes Biology: Embodiment of Social Inequality. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 139(1), 47–57. doi: Doi 10.1002/Ajpa.20983 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Grönniger E, Weber B, Heil O, Peters N, Stäb F, Wenck H, … Lyko F. (2010). Aging and Chronic Sun Exposure Cause Distinct Epigenetic Changes in Human Skin. PLoS Genetics, 6(5), e1000971. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1000971 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Gruenewald TL, Kemeny ME, Aziz N, & Fahey JL (2004). Acute threat to the social self: shame, social self-esteem, and cortisol activity. Psychosomatic Medicine, 66(6), 915–924. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Hahn RA (1995). Sickness and Healing: An Anthropological Perspective. New Haven: Yale University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • Halfon N, & Hochstein M (2002). Life course health development: an integrated framework for developing health, policy, and research. Milbank Q, 80(3), 433–479, iii. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Hallqvist J, Lynch J, Bartley M, Lang T, & Blane D (2004). Can we disentangle life course processes of accumulation, critical period and social mobility? An analysis of disadvantaged socio-economic positions and myocardial infarction in the Stockholm Heart Epidemiology Program. Social Science & Medicine, 58(8), 1555–1562. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(03)00344-7 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Han E, Norton EC, & Stearns SC (2009). Weight and wages: Fat versus lean paychecks. Health Economics 18(5), 535–548. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Harris KM (2010). An Integrative Approach to Health. Demography, 47(1), 1–22. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Harris KM, & Lee H (2011). Social and economic consequences of obesity during the transition to adulthood. Paper presented at the Annual Meetings of the Popualtion Association of America, Washington, D.C. [Google Scholar]
  • Hayward MD, & Gorman BK (2004). The Long Arm of Childhood: The Influence of Early-Life Social Conditions on Men’s Mortality. Demography, 41(1), 87–107. doi: 10.1353/dem.2004.0005 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Heijmans BT, Tobi EW, Stein AD, Putter H, Blauw GJ, Susser ES, … Lumey LH (2008). Persistent epigenetic differences associated with prenatal exposure to famine in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(44), 17046–17049. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0806560105 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Hertzman C, & Boyce T (2010). How experience gets under the skin to create gradients in developmental health. Annu Rev Public Health, 31, 329–347 323p following 347. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.012809.103538 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • House J, Landis KR, & Umberson D (1988). Social relations and health. Science, 241(4865), 540–545. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Hubert HB, Eaker ED, Garrison RJ, Castelli WP (1987). Life-style correlates of risk factor change in young adults: An eight-year study of coronary heart disease risk factors in the Framingham offspring. Am J Epidemiol 125(5), 812–831. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a114598 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Hutter CM, Mechanic LE, Chatterjee N, Kraft P, & Gillanders EM (2013). Gene-Environment Interactions in Cancer Epidemiology: A National Cancer Institute Think Tank Report. Genetic Epidemiology, 37(7), 643–657. doi: 10.1002/gepi.21756 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • (IHEC), International Human Epigenome Consortium (2013). Goals, Structure, Policies & Guidelines.
  • Jablonka E, & Lamb MJ (2002). The changing concept of epigenetics. From Epigenesis to Epigenetics: The Genome in Context, 981, 82–96. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Jablonka E, & Lamb MJ (2015). The inheritance of acquired epigenetic variations. International Journal of Epidemiology, 44(4), 1094–1103. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyv020 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • James PS (2009). Reconstructing Childhood Health Histories. Demography, 46(2), 387–403. doi: 10.1353/dem.0.0058 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Kleinman A (1986). Social Origins of Distress and Disease. New Haven: Yale University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • Krieger N (2005). Embodiment: a conceptual glossary for epidemiology. J Epidemiol Community Health, 59(5), 350–355. doi: 10.1136/jech.2004.024562 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Kuh D, & Ben-Shlomo Y (2004). A Life Course Approach to Chronic Disease Epidemiology. In Ben-Shlomo Y (Ed.), (2nd ed., pp. 473): Oxford University Press (OUP). [Google Scholar]
  • Kuh D, & Ben-Shlomo Y (1997). A life course approach to chronic disease epidemiology. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • Langevin SM, Houseman EA, Christensen BC, Wiencke JK, Nelson HH, Karagas MR, … Kelsey KT (2011). The influence of aging, environmental exposures and local sequence features on the variation of DNA methylation in blood. Epigenetics, 6(7), 908–919. doi: 10.4161/epi.6.7.16431 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Lasker GW (1969). Human biological adaptability: The ecological approach in physical anthropology. Science, 166, 1480–1486. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Lewontin RC, & Levins R (2007). Biology under the influence : dialectical essays on ecology, agriculture, and health. New York: Monthly Review Press. [Google Scholar]
  • Linda W, & Aniruddha D (2010). Families, Social Life, and Well-Being at Older Ages. Demography, 47(S), S87–S109. doi: 10.1353/dem.2010.0009 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Lindau ST, & McDade TW (2007). Minimally invasive and innovative methods for biomeasure collection in population-based research. In Weinstein M, Vaupel JW, Wachter KW & Council NR (Eds.), Biosocial Surveys (pp. 251–277). Washington D.C.: The National Academies Press. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Link BG, & Phelan J (1995). Social Conditions as Fundamental Causes of Disease. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 80–94. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Ludwig J, Sanbonmatsu L, Gennetian L, Adam E, Duncan GJ, Katz LF, … McDade TW (2011). Neighborhoods, Obesity, and Diabetes - A Randomized Social Experiment. New England Journal of Medicine, 365(16), 1509–1519. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Marmot M, Shipley M, Brunner E, & Hemingway H (2001). Relative contribution of early life and adult socioeconomic factors to adult morbidity in the Whitehall II Study. J Epidemiol Community Health 55(5), 301–7. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Marmot M, & Wilkinson R (2005). Social determinants of health (2nd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • McDade TW, K MR, R LF, Arevalo JM, Ma J, Miller GE, & Cole SW (2016). Genome-Wide Profiling of RNA from Dried Blood Spots: Convergence with Bioinformatic Results Derived from Whole Venous Blood and Peripheral Blood Mononuclear Cells. Biodemography Soc Biol, 62(2), 182–197. doi: 10.1080/19485565.2016.1185600 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • McDade TW, Williams S, & Snodgrass JJ (2007). What a drop can do: dried blood spots as a minimally invasive method for integrating biomarkers into population-based research. Demography, 44(4), 899–925. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • McEwen BS (1998). Stress, adaptation and disease: Allostasis and allostatic load. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 840, 33–44. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • McEwen BS, & Lasley EN (2002). The End of Stress as We Know It. New York: Dana Press. [Google Scholar]
  • Miller GE, Chen E, Fok AK, Walker H, Lim A, Nicholls EF, … Kobor MS (2009). Low early-life social class leaves a biological residue manifested by decreased glucocorticoid and increased proinflammatory signaling. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(34), 14716–14721. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0902971106 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Murphy MLM, Slavich GM, Rohleder N, & Miller GE (2012). Targeted Rejection Triggers Differential Pro- and Anti-Inflammatory Gene Expression in Adolescents as a Function of Social Status. Clinical Psychological Science, 1(1), 30–40. doi: 10.1177/2167702612455743 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • National Center for Health Statistics (2016). Health, United States, 2015: With Special Feature on Racial and Ethnic Health Disparities. Hyattsville, MD. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Needham BL, Smith JA, Zhao W, Wang X, Mukherjee B, Kardia SL, … Diez Roux AV (2015). Life course socioeconomic status and DNA methylation in genes related to stress reactivity and inflammation: The multi-ethnic study of atherosclerosis. Epigenetics, 10(10), 958–969. doi: 10.1080/15592294.2015.1085139 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Nguyen QC, Tabor JW, Entzel PP, Lau Y, Suchindran C, Hussey JM, Halpern CT, Harris KM & Whitsel EA (2011). Discordance in National Estimates of Hypertension Among Young Adults. Epidemiology, 22(4), 532–541. doi: 10.1097/ede.0b013e31821c79d2 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • North KE, & Martin LJ (2008). The Importance of Gene--Environment Interaction: Implications for Social Scientists. Sociological Methods & Research, 37(2), 164–200. doi: 10.1177/0049124108323538 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Okbay A, Beauchamp JP, Fontana MA, Lee JJ, Pers TH, Rietveld CA, Turley P et al. (2016). Genome-wide association study identifies 74 loci associated with educational attainment. Nature, 533(7604), 539–542. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Palloni A (2006). Reproducing Inequalities: Luck, Wallets, and the Enduring Effects of Childhood Health. Demography, 43(4), 587–615. doi: 10.1353/dem.2006.0036 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Pearlin LI (1989). The Sociological Study of Stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 30(3), 241. doi: 10.2307/2136956 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Pearlin LI, Menaghan EG, Lieberman MA, & Mullan JT (1981). The Stress Process. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 22(4), 337. doi: 10.2307/2136676 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Phillips DI, Walker BR, Reynolds RM, Flanagan DE, Wood PJ, Osmond C, … Whorwood CB (2000). Low birth weight predicts elevated plasma cortisol concentrations in adults from 3 populations. Hypertension, 35(6), 1301–1306. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Powell B, Hamilton L, Manago B, & Cheng S (2016). Implications of Changing Family Forms for Children. Annual Review of Sociology, 42(1), 301–322. doi: 10.1146/annurev-soc-081715-074444 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Preston SH, Hill ME, & Drevenstedt GL (1998). Childhood conditions that predict survival to advanced ages among African–Americans. Social Science & Medicine, 47(9), 1231–1246. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(98)00180-4 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Pudrovska T, & Anikputa B (2014). Early-life socioeconomic sStatus and mortality in later life: An integration of four life-course mechanisms. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 69(#), 451–60. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Rietveld C, Medland S, Derringer J, Yang J, Esko T, Martin N, … Koellinger P (2013). GWAS of 126,559 individuals identifies genetic variants associated with educational attainment. Science, 340(6139), 1467–1471. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Rowe DC, Jacobson KC, & Van den Oord EJCG (1999). Genetic and Environmental Influences on Vocabulary IQ: Parental Education Level as Moderator. Child Development, 70(5), 1151–1162. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00084 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Scheper-Hughes N, & Lock MM (1987). The mindful body: A prolegomenon to future work in medical anthropology. Medical anthropology quarterly, 1(1), 6–41. [Google Scholar]
  • Schmitz L, & Conley D (2015a). The Long-Term Consequences of Vietnam-Era Conscription and Genotype on Smoking Behavior and Health. Behavior Genetics, 46(1), 43–58. doi: 10.1007/s10519-015-9739-1 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Schmitz L, & Conley D (2015b). Modeling Gene-Environment Interactions With Quasi-Natural Experiments. Journal of Personality, n/a–n/a. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12227 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Seeman TE, McEwen B, Rowe JW, & Singer B (2001). Allostatic load as a marker of cumulative biological risk: MacArthur studies of successful aging. PNAS, 98(8), 4770–4775. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Seligman R (2014). Possessing spirits and healing selves: : embodiment and transformation in an Afro-Brazilian religion. New York, NY: Palgrave Macmillan. [Google Scholar]
  • Shanahan MJ, & Boardman JD (2009). Genetics and behavior in the life course: A promising frontier. The craft of life course research, 215–235. [Google Scholar]
  • Shanahan MJ, Hofer SM, & Shanahan L (2003). Biological Models of Behavior and the Life Course Handbooks of Sociology and Social Research (pp. 597–622): Springer Nature. [Google Scholar]
  • Sisk CL, & Zehr JL (2005). Pubertal hormones organize the adolescent brain and behavior. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 26(3–4), 163–174. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2005.10.003 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Slavich GM, & Cole SW (2013). The Emerging Field of Human Social Genomics. Clinical Psychological Science, 1(3), 331–348. doi: 10.1177/2167702613478594 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Smith CJ, & Ryckman KK (2015). Epigenetic and developmental influences on the risk of obesity, diabetes, and metabolic syndrome. Diabetes, Metabolic Sybndrome and Obesity: Targets and Therapy 8, 295–302. Doi: 10.2147/DMSO.S61296 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Sterling P, & Ayer J (1988). Allostasis: A new paradigm to explain arousal pathology. In Fischer S & Reason J (Eds.), Handbook of Life Stress, Cognition and Health (pp. 629–649). New York: John Wiley and Sons. [Google Scholar]
  • Stinson S, Bogin B, & O’Rourke D (Eds.). (2012). Human Biology: An Evolutionary and Biocultural Perspective. New York: Wiley-Blackwell. [Google Scholar]
  • Sweeney MM, & Raley RK (2014). Race, Ethnicity, and the Changing Context of Childbearing in the United States. Annual Review of Sociology, 40(1), 539–558. doi: 10.1146/annurev-soc-071913-043342 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Thayer ZM, & Kuzawa CW (2011). Biological memories of past environments: epigenetic pathways to health disparities. Epigenetics, 6(7), 798–803. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Tobi EW, Lumey LH, Talens RP, Kremer D, Putter H, Stein AD, … Heijmans BT (2009). DNA methylation differences after exposure to prenatal famine are common and timing- and sex-specific. Human Molecular Genetics, 18(21), 4046–4053. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddp353 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Uchino B, Cacioppo J, & Kiecolt-Glaser J (1996). The relationship between social support and physiological processes: A review with emphasis on underlying mechanisms and implications for health. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 488–531. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Wadhwa PD, Buss C, Entringer S, Swanson JM (2009). Developmental origins of health and disease:brief history of the approach and current focus on epigenetic mechanisms. Semin Reprod Med 27(5), 358–368. [PubMed: 19711246] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Waite L, & Das A (2010). Families, social life, and well-being at older ages. Demography 47 Suppl, S87–109. PMID:21302422 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Ward BW, Schiller JS, & Goodman RA (2014). Multiple Chronic Conditions Among US Adults: A 2012 Update. Preventing Chronic Disease, 11. doi: 10.5888/pcd11.130389 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Weinstein M, Vaupel JW, & Wachter KW (Eds.). (2007). Biosocial Surveys. Washington, D.C.: The National Academies Press. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Wilcox WB, & Lerman RI (2014). For richer, for poorer: How family structures economic success in America. Washington, D.C.: American Enterprise Institute. [Google Scholar]
  • Wolfe B, Evans W, & Seeman T (2012). The Biological Consesquences of Socioeconomic Inequalities. New York: Russell Sage Foundation. [Google Scholar]
  • Yang YC, Boen C, Gerken K, Li T, Schorpp K, & Harris KM (2016). Social relationships and physiological determinants of longevity across the human life span. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 113(3), 578–583. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1511085112 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Yang YC, Gerken K, Schorpp K, Boen C, & Harris KM (2017). Early life socioeconomic status and adult physiological functioning: A life course examination of biosocial mechanisms. Biodemography and Social Biology 63(2): [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Yang YC, Schorpp K, & Harris KM (2014). Social support, social strain and inflammation: Evidence from a national longitudinal study of U.S. adults. Social Science & Medicine, 107, 124–135. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.02.013 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]