Unlike rejected-aggressive children, popular-antisocial children

  • Adams, C. (2002). Practitioner review: the assessment of language pragmatics. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(8), 973–987. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bakhtin, M. M. (1981). The dialogical imagination: four essays. (Trans: Emerson, C. & Holquist, M.). Austin: University of Texas Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bierman, K. L. (2004). Peer rejection. Developmental processes and intervention strategies. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bierman, K. L., & Furman, W. (1984). The effects of social skills training and peer involvement on the social adjustment of preadolescents. Child Development, 55, 151–162. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1984.tb00280.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M. (2014). Pragmatic language impairment: a correlate of SLI, a distinct subgroup, or part of the autistic continuum? In D. V. M. Bishop & L. Leonard (Eds.), Speech and language impairments in children. Causes, characteristics, intervention and outcome (pp. 99–113). London: Psychology Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M. (2017). Why is it so hard to reach agreement on terminology? The case of developmental language disorder (DLD). International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 52(6), 671–680. https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Black, B. (1992). Negotiating social pretend play: communication differences related to social status and sex. Merril-Palmer Quarterly, 38(2), 212–232.

    Google Scholar 

  • Black, B., & Hazen, N. L. (1990). Social status and patterns of communication in acquainted and unacquainted preschool children. Developmental Psychology, 26(3), 379–387. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.26.3.379.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Black, B., & Logan, A. (1995). Links between communication patterns in mother-child, father-child, and child-peer interactions and children’s social status. Child Development, 66(1), 255–271. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00869.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braza, F., Azurmendi, A., Muñoz, J. M., Carreras, M. R., Braza, P., García, A., Sorozabal, A., & Sánchez-Martín, J. R. (2009). Social cognitive predictors of peer acceptance at age 5 and the moderating effects of gender. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 27(3), 703–716. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151008X360666.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bukowski, W. M., Sippola, L. K., Hoza, B., & Newcomb, A. F. (2000). Pages from a sociometric notebook: an analysis of nomination and rating scale measures of acceptance, rejection, and social preference. In T. Cillessen & W. M. Bukowski (Eds.), Recent advances in the study and measurement of acceptance and rejection in the peer system (pp. 11–26). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bukowski, W. M., Cillessen, A. H. N., & Velasquez, A. M. (2012). The use of peer ratings in developmental research. In B. Laursen, T. Little, & N. Card (Eds.), Handbook of developmental research methods (pp. 211–228). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bukowski, W. M., Laursen, B. P., & Rubin, K. H. (2018). Peer relations. Past, present and promise. In W. M. Bukowski, B. P. Laursen, & K. H. Rubin (Eds.), Handbook of Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups (2nd ed., pp. 3–20). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burleson, B. R., Della, J. G., & Applegat, J. L. (1992). Effects of maternal communication and children's social-cognitive and communication skills on children’s acceptance by the peer group. Family Relations, 41(3), 264–272. https://doi.org/10.2307/585189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Celce-Murcia, M. (2008). Rethinking the role of communicative competence in language teaching. In E. A. Soler & P. S. Jordà (Eds.), Intercultural language use and language learning (pp. 41–57). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cillessen, A. H. N. (2009). Sociometric methods. In K. H. Rubin, W. M. Bukowski, & B. Laursen (Eds.), Social, emotional, and personality development in context. Handbook of peer interactions, relationships, and groups (pp. 82–99). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsday, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511499739.006.

  • Coie, J. D., Dodge, K. A., & Coppotelli, H. (1982). Dimensions and types of social status: a cross-age perspective. Developmental Psychology, 18(4), 557–570. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.18.4.557.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dodge, K. A., Lansford, J. E., Salzer Burks, V., Bates, J. E., Pettit, G. S., Fontaine, R., & Price, J. M. (2003). Peer rejection and social information-processing factors in the development of aggressive behavior problems in children. Child Development, 74(2), 374–393. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.7402004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fabes, R. A., Martin, C. L., & Hanish, L. D. (2004). The next 50 years: considering gender as a context for understanding young children’s peer relationships. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 50(3), 260–273. https://doi.org/10.1353/mpq.2004.0017.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • French, D. C., & Conrad, J. (2001). School dropout as predicted by peer rejection and antisocial behavior. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 11(3), 225–244. https://doi.org/10.1111/1532-7795.00011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Galejs, I., Dhawan, G., & King, A. (1983). Popularity and communication skills of preschool children. The Journal of Psychology, 115(1), 89–95. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.1983.9923601.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gifford-Smith, M. E., & Brownell, C. A. (2003). Childhood peer relationships: social acceptance, friendships, and peer networks. Journal of School Psychology, 41(4), 235–284. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-4405(03)00048-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gottman, J., Gonso, J., & Rasmussen, B. (1975). Social interaction, social competence, and friendship in children. Child Development, 46(3), 709–718. https://doi.org/10.2307/1128569.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halliday, M. A. K. (2003). A sociosemiotic perspective on language development. In M. A. K. Halliday (Ed.), (pp. 90–113). New York: Continuum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hay, D. F., Payne, A., & Chadwick, A. (2004). Peer relations in childhood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(1), 84–108. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.0021-9630.2003.00308.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hazen, N. L., & Black, B. (1989). Preschool peer communication skills: the role of social status and interaction context. Child Development, 60(4), 867–876. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1989.tb03519.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henry, D. B. (2006). Associations between peer nominations, teacher ratings, self-reports, and observations of malicious and disruptive behavior. Assessment, 13(3), 241–252. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191106287668.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, J. P. T., & Green, S. (2008). Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions. Chichester: Wiley.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hymes, D. H. (1967). Models of the interaction of language and social setting. Journal of Social Issues, 23(2), 8–28. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.1967.tb00572.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hymes, D. H. (1972). On communicative competence. In J. B. Pride & J. Holmes (Eds.), Sociolinguistics: selected readings (pp. 269–293). Harmondsworth, Penguin.

  • Jiang, X. L., & Cilessen, A. H. N. (2005). Stability of continuous measures of sociometric status: a meta-analysis. Developmental Review, 25(1), 1–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2004.08.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keane, S. P., & Calkins, S. D. (2004). Predicting kindergarten peer social status from toddler and preschool problem behaviour. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 32(4), 409–423. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:JACP.0000030294.11443.41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kemple, K., Speranza, H., & Hazen, N. (1992). Cohesive discourse and peer acceptance: longitudinal relationships in the preschool years. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 38(3), 364–381.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ladd, G. W. (1981). Effectiveness of a social learning method for enhancing children's social interaction and peer acceptance. Child Development, 52(1), 171–178. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laws, G., Bates, G., Feuerstein, M., Mason-Apps, E., & White, C. (2012). Peer acceptance of children with language and communication impairments in a mainstream primary school: associations with type of language difficulty, problem behaviors and a change in placement organization. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 28(1), 73–86. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265659011419234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A. R. (1981). Language and cognition. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maccoby, E. E. (2002). Gender and group process: a developmental perspective. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(2), 54–58. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markell, R. A., & Asher, S. R. (1984). Children’s interactions in dyads: interpersonal influence and sociometric status. Child Development, 55, 1412–1424. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130010.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Masters, J. C., & Furman, W. (1981). Popularity, individual friendship selection, and specific peer interaction among children. Developmental Psychology, 17(3), 344–350. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.17.3.344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Menting, B., van Lier, P. A. C., & Koot, H. M. (2011). Language skills, peer rejection, and the development of externalizing behavior from kindergarten to fourth grade. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(1), 72–79. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.14697610.2010.02279.x.

  • Mercer, S. H., & DeRosier, M. E. (2008). Teacher preference, peer rejection, and student aggression: a prospective study of transactional influence and independent contributions to emotional adjustment and grades. Journal of School Psychology, 46(6), 661–685. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2008.06.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller-Johnson, S., Coie, J. D., Maumary-Gremaud, A., Bierman, K., & the Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. (2002). Peer rejection and aggression and early starter models of conduct disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 30(3), 217–230. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1015198612049.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moreno, J. L. (1934). A new approach to the problem of human interrelations. Washington, DC: Nervous and Mental Disease Publishing.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, S. M., & Faulkner, D. (2000). Learning to collaborate: can young children develop better communication strategies through collaboration with a more popular peer. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 15(4), 389–404. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03172983.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, S. M., & Faulkner, D. (2006). Gender differences in verbal communications between popular and unpopular children during an interactive task. Social Development, 15(1), 82–108. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2006.00331.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, D. A., Burner, K. C., Coyne, S. M., Hart, C. H., & Robinson, C. C. (2016). Correlates of sociometric status in Russian preschoolers: aggression, victimization, and sociability. Personality and Individual Differences, 94, 332–336. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.01.054.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newcomb, A. F., Bukowski, W. M., & Pattee, L. (1993). Children’s peer relations: a meta-analytic review of popular, rejected, neglected, controversial, and average sociometric status. Psychological Bulletin, 113(1), 99–128. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.113.1.99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nowicki, S., & Oxenford, C. (1989). The relation of hostile nonverbal communication styles to popularity in preadolescent children. The Journal of Genetic Psychology: Research and Theory on Human Development, 150(1), 39–44. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221325.1989.9914572.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nærland, T. (2011). Language competence and social focus among preschool children. Early Child Development and Care, 181(5), 599–612. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004431003665780.

  • Nærland, T., & Martinsen, H. (2011). Child–child interactions and positive social focus among preschool children. Early Child Development and Care, 181(3), 361–370. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430903387701.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parker, J. G., Rubin, K. H., Erath, S. A., Wojslawowicz, J. C., & Buskirk, A. A. (2006). Peer relationships, child development, and adjustment: a developmental psychopathology perspective. In D. Cicchetti & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Theory and method (pp. 419–493). Hoboken: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pedersen, S., Vitaro, F., Barker, E. D., & Borge, A. I. (2007). The timing of middle-childhood peer rejection and friendship: linking early behavior to early-adolescent adjustment. Child Development, 78(4), 1037–1051. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01051.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Place, K. S., & Becker, J. A. (1991). The influence of pragmatic competence on the likeability of grade-school children. Discourse Processes, 14(2), 227–241. https://doi.org/10.1080/01638539109544783.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poulin, F., & Dishion, T. J. (2008). Methodological issues in the use of peer sociometric nominations with middle school youth. Social Development, 17(4), 908–921. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2008.00473.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prinstein, M. J., & La Greca, A. M. (2004). Childhood peer rejection and aggression as predictors of adolescent girls’ externalizing and health risk behaviors: a 6-year longitudinal study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72(1), 103–112. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.72.1.103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Putallaz, M. (1983). Predicting children’s sociometric status from their behaviour. Child Development, 54(6), 1417–1426. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129804.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Putallaz, M., & Gottman, J. M. (1981). An interactional model of children’s entry into peer groups. Child Development, 52(3), 986–994. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Putallaz, M., & Wasserman, A. (1989). Children’s naturalistic entry behavior and sociometric status: a developmental perspective. Developmental Psychology, 25(2), 297–305. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.25.2.297.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rabiner, D. L., & Gordon, L. V. (1992). The coordination of conflicting social goals: differences between rejected and nonrejected boys. Child Development, 63(6), 1344–1350. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131560.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Redmond, S. M. (2011). Peer victimization among students with specific language impairment, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, and typical development. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 42(4), 520–535. https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2011/10-0078).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reijntjes, A., Kamphuis, J. H., Prinzie, P., & Telch, M. J. (2010). Peer victimization and internalizing problems in children: a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Child Abuse & Neglect, 34(4), 244–252. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2009.07.009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, F. P., & Spekman, N. J. (1984). Assessing the pragmatic abilities of children: Part 1. Organizational framework and assessment parameters. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 49(1), 2–11. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshd.4901.02.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Samter, W. (2003). Friendship interaction skills across the life span. In J. Greene & P. Burleson (Eds.), Handbook of communication and social interaction skills (pp. 637–684). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shonkhoff, J. P., & Phillips, D. A. (2000) From neurons to neighborhoods: the science of early childhood development. National Research Council, Washington, DC; Institute of Medicine (NAS), Washington, DC.

  • Slavin, R. E. (2008). Perspectives on evidence-based research in education. Educational Researcher, 37(1), 5–14. https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X08314117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Troesch, L. M., Keller, K., & Grob, A. (2016). Language competence and social preference in childhood: a meta-analysis. European Psychologist, 21(3), 167–179. https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040/a000262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vallotton, C., & Ayoub, C. (2011). Use your words: the role of language in the development of toddlers’ self-regulation. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 26(2), 169–181. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2010.09.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van der Wilt, F., van Kruistum, C., van der Veen, C., & van Oers, B. (2016). Gender differences in the relationship between oral communicative competence and peer rejection: An explorative study in early childhood education. European Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 24(6), 807–817. https://doi.org/10.1080/1350293X.2015.1073507.

  • van der Wilt, F., van der Veen, C., van Kruistum, C., & van Oers, B. (2018a). Why can’t I join? Peer rejection in early childhood education and the role of oral communicative competence. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 54, 247–254. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2018.06.007.

  • van der Wilt, F., van der Veen, C., van Kruistum, C., & van Oers, B. (2018b). Popular, rejected, neglected, controversial or average: Do young children of different sociometric groups differ in their level of oral communicative competence? Social Development, 27(4), 793–807. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12316.

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: the development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). Thinking and speech. In R. W. Rieber & J. Wollock (Eds.), The collected works of L.S. Vygotsky. Vol. 1: Problems of general psychology. Including the volume thinking and speech (pp. 39–285). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitebread, D., Pino-Pasternak, D., & Coltman, P. (2015). Making learning visible: the role of language in the development of metacognition and self-regulation in young children. In S. Robson & S. Quinn (Eds.), The Routledge international handbook of young children’s thinking and understanding (pp. 199–214). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wu, X., Hart, C. H., Draper, T. W., & Olsen, J. A. (2001). Peer and teacher sociometrics for preschool children: cross-informant concordance, temporal stability, and reliability. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 47(3), 416–443. https://doi.org/10.1353/mpq.2001.0018.

    Article  Google Scholar 


Page 2

Skip to main content

From: Why Do Children Become Rejected by Their Peers? A Review of Studies into the Relationship Between Oral Communicative Competence and Sociometric Status in Childhood

Study/country Research aim Operationalization Research design Main outcomes
van der Wilt et al. (2016)/The Netherlands To examine gender differences in the relation between oral communicative competence and peer rejection Nijmegen Test for Pragmatics, assessing productive pragmatic skills individually in a standardized manner N = 54 children (age range 3.83 to 6.25 years); correlation analyses; cross-sectional design Oral communicative competence was associated with peer acceptance (although only for boys), but not with peer rejection.
Peer nomination procedure: amount of received positive nominations indicated acceptance; amount of received negative nominations indicated rejection
van der Wilt et al. (2018a)/The Netherlands To examine the relation between oral communicative competence and peer rejection, as well as gender differences in this relation Nijmegen Test for Pragmatics, assessing productive pragmatic skills individually in a standardized manner N = 447 children (age range 3.83 to 6.50 years); multiple regression analyses; cross-sectional design After controlling for gender, age, and SES, oral communicative competence accounted for unique variance: children with poorer oral communicative competence experienced higher levels of peer rejection.
Peer nomination procedure: amount of received negative nominations minus amount of received positive nominations indicated level of peer rejection
van der Wilt et al. (2018b)/The Netherlands To examine whether children who differed in their sociometric status also differed in their level of oral communicative competence Nijmegen Test for Pragmatics, assessing productive pragmatic skills individually in a standardized manner N = 570 children (age range 3.99 to 7.08 years); comparative analyses; cross-sectional design Children who were rejected or neglected by their peers exhibited lower levels of oral communicative competence than average children; popular and controversial children did not differ from average children in their level of oral communicative competence.
Peer nomination procedure resulting in five groups: (1) popular, (2) rejected, (3) neglected, (4) controversial, and (5) average
Bierman and Furman (1984)/USA To examine the effects of conversational skills training and peer involvement on the peer acceptance of disliked children Observations of peer interactions and a written questionnaire measuring different aspects of communication N = 56 children (fifth- and sixth graders); comparative analyses; quasi-experimental, longitudinal design Conversational skills training promoted social skill acquisition and increased skillful social interaction in both dyadic and small-group interactions; peer involvement increased peer acceptance
Rating scale resulting in a measure of peer acceptance
Black (1992)/USA To examine the communication strategies that children use to negotiate episodes of social pretend play as a function of social status and sex Observations of peer interactions in which one child enters the play of others N = 68 children (age range 3.58 to 5.08 years); comparative analyses; cross-sectional design Disliked children used higher proportions of suggestions and demands, were more likely to negotiate pretense in terms of their own activity, and were more likely to reject a theme or role; liked children provided explanations about ongoing play, requested clarifications, and included the ideas of peers in their negotiations.
Peer nomination procedure resulting in three groups: (1) liked, (2) disliked, and (3) low-impact
Black and Hazen (1990)/USA To examine the relation between social communication skills and sociometric status Observations of peer interactions in which one child enters the play session of two peers he or she did or did not know N = 66 children (age range 3.50 to 5.33 years); comparative analyses; cross-sectional design With unacquainted peers, disliked children were less responsive to peers and more likely to make irrelevant comments; with acquainted peers, disliked children were also less likely to clearly direct their communication.
Peer nomination procedure resulting in three groups: (1) liked, (2) disliked, (3) low-impact
Black and Logan (1995)/USA To examine links between specific aspects of communication in the family and peer system and children’s sociometric status Observations of peer interactions in which one child enters the play session of two peers N = 43 children (age range 2.00 to 5.00 years); comparative analyses; cross-sectional design Rejected children demonstrated turn-taking styles that included irrelevant turns, interruptions, simultaneous talking, and non-contingent responding; popular children alternated turns, provided explanations to peers, and were more likely to display coherent discourse
Teachers provided peer nominations, resulting in four groups: (1) popular, (2) rejected, (3) neglected, and (4) controversial
Burleson et al. (1992)/USA To examine four of children’s socio-cognitive skills and three of their communication skills as potential predictors of peer acceptance Coding of children’s responses to hypothetical situations, resulting in a general communication skill index N = 51 children (first- and third graders); multiple regression analyses; longitudinal design (year 1 and year 2) During year 1, persuasive communication predicted social preference; during year 2, persuasive communication and listener-adapted communication predicted social preference
Peer nomination procedure: amount of received positive nominations minus amount of received negative nominations indicated social preference
Galejs et al. (1983)/USA To examine the relation between popularity and communication skills Dickson’s Notebook Communication Game, assessing listening and describing skills N = 60 children (age range 3.20 to 6.60 years); correlation analyses; cross-sectional design A significant and positive correlation was found between popularity and listening skills; a non-significant correlation was found between popularity and describing skills
Teachers rank-ordered all children from most popular to least popular
Gottman et al. (1975) / USA To examine the relations between social skills, social interaction, and popularity Observations of peer interactions in the classroom N = 198 children (third- and fourth graders); comparative analyses; cross-sectional design High-friend children received more positive reinforcement compared to low-friend children
Peer nomination procedure (nominating unlimited amount of friends) resulting in two categories: low-friends children and high-friends children
Hazen and Black (1989)/USA To examine the relation between social status and discourse skills Observations of peer interactions in which one child enters the play session of two peers N = 48 children (age range 3.58 to 5.50 years); comparative analyses; cross-sectional design Liked children were more likely to direct their initiations clearly, speak to both interaction partners, respond contingently to others, acknowledge others, reinitiate when rejecting, and adapt to the differing social demands of different contexts than disliked children
Peer nomination procedure resulting in three groups: (1) liked, (2) disliked, and (3) low-impact
Kemple et al. (Kemple et al. 1992)/USA To examine relations between social status and communication behaviors contributing to cohesive discourse longitudinally Observations of peer interactions in which one child enters the play session of others N = 25 children (age range 3.50 to 4.50 years); correlation analyses; longitudinal design (two measurements: year 1 and year 2) Acceptance of peers’ initiations during play at year 1 predicted social status at year 2; social status at year 1 predicted use of non-directed initiations and non-contingent responding at year 2
Peer nomination procedure; amount of received positive nominations indicated acceptance; amount of received negative nominations indicated rejection
Ladd (1981)/USA To examine the effect of a social skills training on the behavior and acceptance of low-accepted children Observations of children’s peer interactions during 30-min free-play periods N = 36 low-accepted children (third graders); comparative analyses; quasi-experimental, longitudinal design Trained children spent a significantly greater percentage of tune engagement in two of the three trained skills whereas control group children remained the same or declined; trained children also evidenced significant and lasting gains in classroom peer acceptance
Rating scale resulting in a selection of low-accepted children
Markell and Asher (1984)/USA To examine whether popular and unpopular children differ in their interaction style in a problem-solving situation Observations of peer interactions during a problem-solving situation N = 208 children (third- and fourth graders); comparative analyses; cross-sectional design Pairs of unpopular and average children showed greater asymmetry in influence and less cohesiveness and comfort than pairs of popular-average children
Peer nomination procedure and peer rating scale
Masters and Furman (1981)/USA To examine the role of specific peer interactions in children’s sociometric status and friendship choices Observations of peer interactions during free-play periods N = 94 children (age range 4.00 to 5.00 years); correlation analyses and comparative analyses; cross-sectional design Popularity was associated with receiving and dispensing reinforcing and neutral acts; children’s interactions with disliked peers did not differ from those with peers who were neither liked nor disliked
Peer nomination procedure; amount of received positive nominations indicates popularity and amount of received negative nominations indicates unpopularity
Murphy and Faulkner (2000)/UK To examine whether pairing unpopular children with a popular peer would promote more effective communication Observations of peer interaction while children were playing a collaborative game N = 72 children (age range 4.00 to 6.00 years); comparative analyses; quasi-experimental design Collaboration between popular children and popular peers was more successful and less disputational than collaboration between unpopular children and unpopular peers; unpopular children were more successful when collaborating with popular peers.
Peer nomination procedure and peer rating scale
Murphy and Faulkner (2006)/UK To examine gender differences in communication effectiveness between pairs of popular and unpopular children Observations of peer interactions while children were playing a game N = 48 children (age range 5.00 to 7.00 years); comparative analyses; quasi-experimental design Popular girls used a greater incidence of speech forms associated with successful collaboration than unpopular girls; there was no difference between popular boys and unpopular boys.
Peer nomination procedure and peer rating scale
Nærland (2011)/Norway To examine the relation between social focus and pragmatic skills Observations of peer interactions during free-play N = 64 children (age range 0.91 to 5.08 years); correlation analyses and regression analyses; cross-sectional design Pragmatic skills had an independent contribution to social focus when the variance of age was removed.
Observations of the amount of positive and neutral contacts children received from their peers
Nærland and Martinsen (2011)/Norway To examine the extent to which the social focus children receive from their peers can be explained by aspects of peer interaction Observations of peer interactions during free-play resulting in a contact score (reflecting the content, manner and social function of the interactions) N = 64 children (age range 0.92 to 5.08 years); correlation analyses and regression analyses; cross-sectional design Twelve out of 16 contact categories were significantly correlated with social focus; combined, they predicted children’s social focus to a large extent
Observations of the amount of positive and neutral contacts children received from their peers
Nowicki and Oxenford (1989)/USA To examine the relation between communication styles and popularity Observations of peer interactions while children were planning a daytrip together N = 62 children (fifth- and sixth graders); comparative analyses; cross-sectional design Compared with popular children, unpopular children had communication styles characterized by non-verbal visual hostility; no differences were found in verbal communication styles between popular and unpopular children.
Peer nomination procedure: the number of times a child was chosen as most preferred minus the number of times a child was chosen as least preferred indicated popularity
Place and Becker (1991)/USA To examine the impact of pragmatic skills on likeability Audiotapes of a girl using four different pragmatic skills either appropriately or inappropriately in a conversation with a school librarian N = 91 girls (third- and fourth graders); comparative analyses; experimental design Children saw the girl of the audiotape as more likeable when she displayed pragmatic competence than when she requested inappropriately, interrupted and failed to maintain the logic of the conversation.
Rating scale indicating how much children would like to play with the girl of the audiotape
Putallaz (1983)/USA To examine the relation between children’s communicative behavior and their sociometric status Observations of children’s entry behavior in serval game situations N = 22 boys (first graders); multiple regression analyses; experimental design The tendency of subjects to fit into the group they entered by contributing to relevant conversation was predictive of their sociometric status (after controlling for intelligence).
Rating scale indicating how much children liked to play with a target child
Putallaz and Gottman (1981)/USA To examine differences between the dyadic interactions of popular versus unpopular children Videotaped observations of children’s peer interactions while they were playing a game N = 60 children (second- and third graders); comparative analyses; cross-sectional design Unpopular children were more disagreeable and less likely to provide a general reason or rule for their disagreement or to suggest a constructive alternative when criticizing a peer.
Peer nomination procedure resulting in two groups: popular and unpopular children
Putallaz and Wasserman (1989)/USA To examine the relation between children’s entry behavior and sociometric status Observations of children’s naturalistic entry behavior over a 15-week period N = 72 children (first- third-, and fifth graders); comparative analyses; cross-sectional design Low-status children engaged in more passive entry attempts and less sustained group interaction than high-status children
Combination of peer nomination procedure (only positive nominations) and rating scale resulting in three groups: high-status child, average-status child, low-status child
Rabiner and Gordon (1992)/USA To examine differences between rejected and non-rejected boys in their social interaction strategies Six short vignettes describing children in potentially conflictual interactions were used and children were asked to respond and tell what social interaction strategies they would use in the social dilemma N = 58 boys (fourth- and fifth graders); comparative analyses; cross-sectional design Aggressive rejected boys and residual rejected boys provided less integrated responses than non-rejected boys.
Peer nomination procedure resulting in three groups: popular, average, and rejected